Best Life Insider

A top psychologist says there's only one way to become the best in your field. Not everyone agrees.

violinist

  • Anders Ericsson is an expert on experts, who studies exactly how people reach peak performance.
  • In decades of research, he has found that just practicing isn't enough. To truly improve, you must engage in what Ericsson calls "deliberate practice."
  • Other scientists disagree, saying that factors like intelligence and innate skill matter, too.
  • Click here for more BI Prime stories. 

As a teenager in Sweden, Anders Ericsson used to play chess against one of his classmates, a boy considerably worse at the game than Ericsson. Every time they'd play, Ericsson would trounce him.

Then one day, the classmate beat him.

Ericsson wanted to know: What exactly had the boy done to improve his performance so drastically?

Though Ericsson didn't realize it then, the question would come to define his life's work.

In the years that followed his defeat at the hands of his classmate, Ericsson found himself less interested in improving at chess and more interested in learning how people improve at anything.

Today, Ericsson is a professor of psychology at Florida State University, where he specializes, among many topics, in the science of peak performance. He is, in other words, an expert on experts.

Ericsson's research suggests that, if you spend enough time on a very structured practice regimen, you'll eventually excel at that skill.

This perspective is hardly universally accepted. In the last few years, Ericsson's research has come under intense scrutiny from other social scientists, who say Ericsson's findings are viscerally appealing because they suggest that anyone can do anything — but that's not true.

Deliberate practice means stepping outside your comfort zone

anders ericssonAccording to Ericsson's research and logic, the sole reason you aren't a virtuoso violinist, or an Olympic athlete, or another kind of world-class performer, is that you haven't engaged in a process he calls "deliberate practice."

In general, according to Ericsson, deliberate practice involves stepping outside your comfort zone and trying activities beyond your current abilities. While repeating a skill you've already mastered might be satisfying, it's not enough to help you get better. Moreover, simply wanting to improve isn't enough — people also need well-defined goals and the help of a teacher who makes a plan for achieving them.

At first, the teacher gives feedback on your efforts; eventually, you can spot problems in your own performance and change it accordingly. Ericsson's research has led him to study expert spellers, elite athletes, and memory champions — and he attributes their diverse successes to deliberate practice.

Most notably, Ericsson's work on deliberate practice formed the basis for the "10,000-hour rule" featured in Malcolm Gladwell's 2008 book, "Outliers": Put in about 10,000 hours of practice, and you'll become an expert.

But Ericsson said Gladwell misinterpreted his research and that 10,000 hours of merely repeating the same activity over and over again is not sufficient to catapult someone to the top of their field.

Gladwell likely drew his conclusions from a paper that Ericsson and his colleagues published in 1993, which studied 40 violinists in Germany. Specifically, the researchers focused on what factors differentiated the best musicians from the good or the mediocre ones.

After asking all the violinists to give detailed records of how they spent their time, on music practice as well as on other activities, the researchers found that the top two groups of violinists spent significantly more hours practicing alone than the others did. The researchers also studied pianists and yielded similar conclusions.

On average, the best violinists had spent a total of 10,000 hours practicing their craft by 20 years old. Some, however, had practiced much more, or less, than that.

Ericsson's findings from the past three decades suggest that deliberate practice is the key to achieving high levels of performance in any field.

Ericsson doesn't believe IQ is as important as the right kind of effort

chessIn his 2016 book, "Peak: Secrets From the New Science of Expertise," cowritten with the journalist Robert Pool, Ericsson translates the principles of deliberate practice from academic-speak into plain language.

When I spoke with Ericsson by phone, he told me that people who think practice can only get you so far aren't talking about the same kind of practice as he is.

As for whether genetic differences — say, in cognitive or physical ability — account for variations in achievement, Ericsson is skeptical.

Height and body size, he said, are the only genetically predetermined traits that can't be altered much through practice and have a meaningful impact on performance.

Even intelligence, Ericsson said, is not directly linked to expert performance. In "Peak," he cites a study by British researchers, which found that intelligence does indeed predict chess skill among children. But when those researchers looked only at kids who were elite chess players, a higher IQ was linked to worse skills.

While a high IQ helps kids learn the game and acquire the basic skills required to play, once other kids catch up intelligence level probably doesn't matter, Ericsson explains. But deliberate practice does.

"I've been spending now 30 years trying to look for kind of limits that would actually constrain some individuals from being successful in some domain," Ericsson told me. "And I'm surprised that I've yet really to find such limits."

Many scientists disagree with Ericsson's conclusions

In 2019, researchers at Case Western Reserve University tried and failed to replicate Ericsson's 1993 findings on deliberate practice. The researchers studied violinists and found that the most skilled among them had not spent more time engaged in deliberate practice. In fact, the researchers report, most of the best violinists had spent less time practicing than the mediocre violinists.

Brooke Macnamara, who led the 2019 research, also published a paper on the limits of deliberate practice in 2016. Macnamara and colleagues analyzed the results of thousands of studies and found that while deliberate practice did account for some of the differences between more and less skilled athletes, it couldn't account for all of them.

The researchers can't say for sure what other factors matter, but they propose that genetically influenced physical traits such as how easily you build muscle mass, and even psychological traits like confidence, play a role.

In a previous interview with Business Insider, Macnamara said the concept of deliberate practice appeals to people because "it's very 'American Dream.'"

But, she said, it's an "oversimplification." Even if you improve at a particular skill with practice, you may never get better than someone who practices less. Macnamara said our ability to do anything — play the violin, tap dance, swim — depends on "a combination of genetic factors, environmental factors, and their interactions."

In a response to Macnamara's 2016 paper, Ericsson wrote that these conclusions are inaccurate, largely because they conflate total hours of accumulate practice with deliberate practice specifically.

Unless you're performing exercises that were assigned by a teacher to help you improve in a particular area, Ericsson believes you're not engaging in deliberate practice. Plenty of practice activities are completely ineffective and won't lead to improvement, he told me.

Most scientists agree that that practice is important, even if it's not all-important

Charles DickensReading over the scientific studies on deliberate practice, I couldn't help but wonder what these findings mean for professional writers.

Given the right practice conditions, could I really become the next Charles Dickens, or maybe Taffy Brodesser-Akner?

Based on my understanding of Ericsson's research, it would take a lifetime of adherence to rigid practice criteria — but yes, it would theoretically be possible.

I posed a similar question to Zach Hambrick, a professor of psychology at Michigan State University. Hambrick coauthored the 2016 paper questioning Ericsson's findings, and another analysis that suggests traits like intelligence could be just as meaningful as deliberate practice.

Hambrick said he didn't know whether you could practice your way to eloquence. To date, no research has explored the role of deliberate practice in writing expertise. But Hambrick also said he'd be surprised if writing ability was entirely dependent on deliberate practice. In other words, unless I started out with certain requisite abilities, a lifetime might not be enough for me to achieve Dickens or Brodesser-Akner levels.

Hambrick, however, kept reiterating that practice is important, if not essential, to the development of expertise in any field.

"I don't think there's any downside to believing in the power of practice," he said. "I do think there is a possible downside to sort of thinking that anybody can accomplish anything with no limits."

To put it frankly, believing that people can deliberately practice their way to peak performance might be a waste of time.

"People might start training in things and end up not improving much when they could have invested their resources and energies and time and training in other things which they might be more likely to excel at," Hambrick said.

You may be able to achieve expertise in only one area

Beyond whether deliberate practice leads directly to expertise, perhaps the insight that struck me most in "Peak" was this: To become an expert, you may need to be willing to sacrifice short-term pleasure for potential satisfaction of success down the road. A key tenet of deliberate practice is that it's generally not enjoyable.

Instead, it's about doing things that don't come naturally or easily, which can be tough. "Practice really involves failing a lot until you eventually reach your goal," Ericsson told me.

He cited research on figure skaters (which appeared in this book) that found elite skaters spent more time than average ones practicing jumps and spins in routines they hadn't yet mastered. By contrast, average skaters spent more time going over routines they were already good at.

So what's the downside to becoming an expert? Perhaps it's that you need to devote yourself wholly to one area — that while one door flies wide open, others slowly creak to a close. Ericsson said he doesn't know of anyone who's become a world-class expert in more than one skill.

That's why, he said, becoming an expert in anything and placing 100% focus on becoming the best in one domain "may not be basically the right thing here for even the majority of people."

That is to say, there's nothing inherently wrong with being average. In fact, working toward expertise in any area can be a grueling, lonely, and often ugly undertaking.

Ericsson, however, does practice what he preaches. He said he personally uses the principles of deliberate practice to become a better researcher. Every article he published, as a postdoctoral researcher and afterward, was the result of multiple drafts and feedback from the psychologists he was working with.

Ericsson cited one 1980 paper, which he cowrote with the late cognitive psychologist Bill Chase and published in the journal Science.

"Partly because I am Swedish," Ericsson said, "it took over 30 different drafts with comments on problems and issues by Bill before we had an article that Bill was willing add his name to."

SEE ALSO: Microsoft is rolling out a new management framework to its leaders. It centers around a psychological insight called growth mindset.

Join the conversation about this story »

NOW WATCH: A sports psychologist reveals the key to ending a slump

Visit dearJulius.com to get free premium content for all of your lifestyle needs.

COMMENTS



Name

Advice,97,Balance Work & Life,50,Be a Better Manager,33,Break Room,12,Business Skills,102,Career,1547,Career Advice,298,Career Choice,125,Career Growth,180,Career Paths,24,Career Problem,8,Education,84,Entrepreneurship,39,Featured,39,Features,152,Finance,4,Internship,8,Interview Skills,37,Job Search,47,Leadership,44,Marketing,10,Money & Career,21,Resume,11,Tools & Skills,16,Training,1,Work Environment,77,
ltr
item
Career - Best Life Insider: A top psychologist says there's only one way to become the best in your field. Not everyone agrees.
A top psychologist says there's only one way to become the best in your field. Not everyone agrees.
https://static4.businessinsider.com/image/554b8bed6da81106194f7fa1-1149/violinist-1.jpg
Career - Best Life Insider
https://career-bestlifeinsider.blogspot.com/2019/12/a-top-psychologist-says-theres-only-one.html
https://career-bestlifeinsider.blogspot.com/
https://career-bestlifeinsider.blogspot.com/
https://career-bestlifeinsider.blogspot.com/2019/12/a-top-psychologist-says-theres-only-one.html
true
4349954968455113299
UTF-8
Loaded All Posts Not found any posts VIEW ALL Read More Reply Cancel reply Delete By Home PAGES POSTS View All RECOMMENDED FOR YOU LABEL ARCHIVE SEARCH ALL POSTS Not found any post match with your request Back Home Sunday Monday Tuesday Wednesday Thursday Friday Saturday Sun Mon Tue Wed Thu Fri Sat January February March April May June July August September October November December Jan Feb Mar Apr May Jun Jul Aug Sep Oct Nov Dec just now 1 minute ago $$1$$ minutes ago 1 hour ago $$1$$ hours ago Yesterday $$1$$ days ago $$1$$ weeks ago more than 5 weeks ago Followers Follow THIS PREMIUM CONTENT IS LOCKED STEP 1: Share to a social network STEP 2: Click the link on your social network Copy All Code Select All Code All codes were copied to your clipboard Can not copy the codes / texts, please press [CTRL]+[C] (or CMD+C with Mac) to copy Table of Content